Commentary What are big brains for ?

نویسنده

  • Dorothy L. Cheney
چکیده

A article in this issue of PNAS by Reader and Laland (1) offers a novel answer to the question ‘‘Why do primates have such big brains?’’ Across the animal kingdom, brain size increases with increasing body size. Despite this common scaling principle, however, brain size to body weight ratios differ from one taxonomic group to another (2). In primates, for example, the brains of apes are generally larger relative to body weight than the brains of monkeys, whereas the brains of monkeys are larger than those of prosimians (2). Structural differences are also apparent. In chimpanzees, a larger proportion of the brain is devoted to neocortex than in monkeys, who in turn have proportionately more neocortex than prosimians (3, 4). Within the neocortex, ape (and especially human) brains have a particularly enlarged prefrontal cortex, an area known to be involved in many forms of abstract thought and rule learning (5, 6). Increases in the size of primate brains have come despite the fact that brain tissue is metabolically very costly (7). What selective pressures have overcome these costs? When the question is applied to humans answers typically refer to the adaptive advantages of technology (initially, stone tools) and language. But monkeys and apes use only rudimentary tools and lack language entirely, yet their brains are significantly larger than those of similar-sized mammals. Some other selective pressures must be at work. Among primates, relative brain size (corrected for body weight) is greater in species with larger home ranges and greater in species that are fruit-eating or omnivorous than in species that eat leaves (8). Species that feed on fruit may face special problems in learning and memory because they depend on widely spaced food that is ephemeral in both space and time (9, 10). In contrast to this ‘‘ecological’’ explanation of brain evolution, others suggest that primate brains have evolved primarily to deal with social problems. Primates, they argue, live in relatively large groups where an individual’s survival and reproductive success depends on its ability to manipulate others within a complex web of kinship and dominance relations (11– 13). In recent years this ‘‘social intelligence’’ hypothesis has received two sorts of empirical support. First, several authors have shown that, in both primates and carnivores, neocortex size is positively correlated with group size, independent of a species’ home range size (14). Group size is here taken as a ‘‘proxy for social complexity.’’ Second, Harcourt (15, 16) found that primates do indeed differ from most other species in at least one measure of social complexity, patterns of alliances. Alliances occur whenever two animals, A and B, are involved in an aggressive encounter and a third, C, joins the fight in support of one of them. An alliance may be formed spontaneously, or it may occur only after C has been solicited by A or B. Alliances occur in many species, primarily among close relatives. Only primates, however, form their alliances ‘‘strategically,’’ selectively soliciting support from some individuals more than from others. And only primates compete to establish close bonds with particular powerful partners (16). In many species, for example, animals compete to form bonds with the highestranking individuals and preferentially solicit as allies those who rank higher than both themselves and their opponent (17– 19). To solicit partners in this way, an individual must know not only its own relative rank but also the rank relations that exist among others. Such ‘‘triadic’’ knowledge can be obtained only by observing interactions in which one is not involved and making the appropriate deductions (20). Moreover, as group size increases, the need for triadic knowledge places increasing demands on individuals, because larger groups produce an explosive growth in the number of triadic relations (21). If primates are, in fact, unique in forming strategic alliances, and if strategic alliances require knowledge of the relations that exist among others, then the social competition found in large groups offers one explanation—unrelated to tools or language—for primates’ unusually large brains. The purported link between brain size and ecological or social intelligence is, however, entirely conjectural. We may assume that memorizing the location of ripe fruit or remembering the kin relations of ones’ opponents demand considerable brainpower, but this assumption is neither supported nor refuted by any widely accepted evidence. Perhaps more important, the ‘‘intelligence’’ of different species is notoriously difficult to compare. Different species manifest their intelligence in different ways, making it almost impossible to find an objective measure of intelligent performance that can be used across many taxa (22). In the current issue, Simon Reader and Kevin Laland (1) offer a novel approach to research on brain and intelligence in primates. Following the pioneering work of Lefebvre et al. (23) on birds, Reader and Laland searched all of the major primate journals for evidence of innovation (defined as apparently novel solutions to environmental or social problems), social learning (the acquisition of information from others), and tool use. They assumed that the frequency of such behaviors, appropriately corrected for the amount of time that had been devoted to studying each species, would provide a useful measure of a species’ behavioral f lexibility, and that behavioral f lexibility was a good measure of intelligence. Once they had accumulated data on innovation, social learning, and tool use from 116 primate species, Reader and Laland tested whether the frequency of such behavior was correlated with brain size. They found significant, positive correlations between brain size and all three behaviors. Reader’s and Laland’s results offer a new perspective on the social intelligence hypothesis because they found no significant relation between group size and the frequency of social learning. Natural selection may, therefore, have favored an increase in brain size because of benefits derived from innovation or social learning that are independent of a species’ typical group size. Reader’s and Laland’s analysis also reminds us that ecological and social intelligence are difficult to distinguish in present-day species and unlikely to have played entirely separate roles during evo-

برای دانلود متن کامل این مقاله و بیش از 32 میلیون مقاله دیگر ابتدا ثبت نام کنید

ثبت نام

اگر عضو سایت هستید لطفا وارد حساب کاربری خود شوید

منابع مشابه

The question what of subject-based commentary and its difference with Quran to Quran commentary

Subject-based commentary is one of the methods more used by commentators in recent decades than before. In this method, commentators may take a Quranic subject on the basis of inter-Quranic or metaphysical fields, or on the grounds of individual and social life. This method differs from that of Quran to Quran. The latter would lead to explanation and deep perception of the meaning of Quranic wo...

متن کامل

Postpartum Haemorrhage: Still a Big Issue in Maternity Care - What is Going Wrong?

Background and aim: Postpartum haemorrhage remains one of the main leading causes of maternal mortality across the world. This is despite a wide-spread coverage of active third stage of labour care in many countries.  Reflecting on emerging evidence in this context, a discussion of associated factors which should be considered in interpretation of the evidence and its implications is presented ...

متن کامل

Adopting New International Health Instruments – What Can We Learn From the FCTC?; Comment on “The Legal Strength of International Health Instruments - What It Brings to Global Health Governance?”

This Commentary forms a response to Nikogosian’s and Kickbusch’s forward-looking perspective about the legal strength of international health instruments. Building on their arguments, in this commentary we consider what we can learn from the Framework Convention on Tobacco Control (FCTC) for the adoption of new legal international health instruments.

متن کامل

An Architecture for Security and Protection of Big Data

The issue of online privacy and security is a challenging subject, as it concerns the privacy of data that are increasingly more accessible via the internet. In other words, people who intend to access the private information of other users can do so more efficiently over the internet. This study is an attempt to address the privacy issue of distributed big data in the context of cloud computin...

متن کامل

Big Data and the Fabric of Human Geography

Digital data tracking what we do, the time and place of our actions, and the chains of interdependence that link those actions together, help us draw a richer picture of human geography as it unfolds in its multiple layers. This commentary briefly illustrates the type of maps and models we can build with that data as well as some important challenges that arise from their complexity and unsolve...

متن کامل

Big-brained birds survive better in nature.

Big brains are hypothesized to enhance survival of animals by facilitating flexible cognitive responses that buffer individuals against environmental stresses. Although this theory receives partial support from the finding that brain size limits the capacity of animals to behaviourally respond to environmental challenges, the hypothesis that large brains are associated with reduced mortality ha...

متن کامل

ذخیره در منابع من


  با ذخیره ی این منبع در منابع من، دسترسی به آن را برای استفاده های بعدی آسان تر کنید

برای دانلود متن کامل این مقاله و بیش از 32 میلیون مقاله دیگر ابتدا ثبت نام کنید

ثبت نام

اگر عضو سایت هستید لطفا وارد حساب کاربری خود شوید

عنوان ژورنال:

دوره   شماره 

صفحات  -

تاریخ انتشار 2002